Skip to main content

Or 34,00 After 66% tax deduction

I make a monthly donation I make an IFI donation
Research, science & health

Science of dreams

Published on: 16/10/2015 Reading time: 1 min
rêves

Very few people pretend they have never dreamt. Pr. Isabelle Arnulf’s work, Head of sleep disorders at the Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital and researcher at Paris Brain Institute, and her colleagues, prove that this is false. Everybody dreams, but some do not remember. Let’s focus on these non-dreamers...

Kill dragons, fly over oceans, be chased by monsters, get naked in public, reset exams... The dream is an altered state of consciousness in which the brain constructs a virtual world of images and striking sensations that we do not immediately identify as hallucinations. Some people have fascinating dream lives; however, a small proportion of the population does not remember their dreams and some even pretend they have never dreamt!

Pr. Isabelle Arnulf, a researcher at the Institut du Cerveau - ICM, works with patients with Parkinson’s disease. Most of these patients “live their dreams”, which means that, even asleep, they can talk, shout, laugh, cry or gesticulate. These behavioural disorders during paradoxical sleep (when we dream the most) also happen in healthy subjects with no other symptoms. As in the general population, around 3% of people suffering from these behavioural disorders during paradoxical sleep pretend that they never dream. Isabelle Arnulf’s team wanted to understand why.

BECAUSE OF MEMORY TROUBLES?

The researchers first wondered whether these patients simply had memory troubles. Different tests showed that they had the same level of memory and the same cognitive profile that those who remember their dreams. Would it thus be possible not to dream?

BECAUSE THEY DO NOT HAVE MENTAL IMAGING?

Researchers checked that these non-dreamer people were capable of mental imaginations, that they could for instance imagine the Eiffel Tower with their eyes closed: no deficit there either, no “afantasie” (inability to visualize a mental image, a rare neurological disorder).

BECAUSE THEY DO NOT DREAM?

By studying the sleep of these “non-dreamers” and observing their nocturnal activity in laboratory, researchers highlighted a dreamlike behaviour through act or speech overnight, indicating that patients are actually dreaming. Some live a violent argument, others smoke a cigarette... These dreams are clearly marked for an outside observer, yet none of the patients remembers these upon awakening, even though this awakening occurs during paradoxical sleep.

BECAUSE DREAMS DO NOT PRINT ON THEIR MEMORY?

If “non-dreamers” dream, the most likely hypothesis is that they do not remember because their dreams are not encoded, printed in their memory during sleep. In order to test this hypothesis, the next step consists in comparing the brain activity of “non-dreamers” to that of “dreamers” through functional brain imaging. This study could help understand what happens in the brain while dreaming and how this gets printed in the memory. Another study would consist in exploring whether dreams are printed (encoded) but not recalled: by giving “non-dreamers” clues about their dreams through video recordings (a word, a situation), in order to determine whether this helps them remember.

Behavioural disorders in paradoxical sleep are a great tool to study dreams and states of consciousness. The work of Isabelle Arnulf’s team show that everybody dreams, even though some people do not remember. The problem would be how dreams get collected and stored by the brain, without any further impairment of memory.

Our news on the subject

Interneurones. Crédit : UCLA Broad Stem Cell Research Center.
Stimulating specific neurons in the striatum stops compulsive behaviour
What if we could resist compulsions? These irrational behaviours, particularly common in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), are hard to suppress. At Paris Brain Institute, Éric Burguière's team shows that we can anticipate them and block them...
09.10.2024 Research, science & health
Les nerfs moteurs présents dans la moelle épinière se projettent vers la périphérie, où ils entrent en contact avec les muscles, formant des connexions appelées jonctions neuromusculaires. Crédit : James N. Sleigh.
Ultrasound show unexpected effects on motor neuron disease
Over the past fifteen years, neurosurgeons have been perfecting a fascinating technique: using ultrasound to temporarily open the blood-brain barrier to facilitate the action of therapeutic molecules in the central nervous system. At Paris Brain...
09.05.2024 Research, science & health
Un neurone
Rett syndrome: a new gene therapy on the way
Gene therapy could be our best chance of treating Rett syndrome, a neurological disorder that causes severe intellectual and motor impairments. At Paris Brain Institute, Françoise Piguet and her colleagues have looked closely at brain cholesterol...
07.16.2024 Research, science & health
Lésions d’un patient à l’inclusion dans le protocole (M0) disparues après 2 ans de traitement à la Leriglitazone (M24)
The dual effect of leriglitazone in X-linked Adrenoleukodystrophy (X-ALD)
In 2023, the team led by Professor Fanny Mochel (AP-HP, Sorbonne University), a Paris brain Institute researcher, showed that daily dose of leriglitazone slow down the progression of myelopathy in patients with X-linked adrenoleukodystrophy, and...
06.28.2024 Research, science & health
Une tête de statue de l'île de Pâques sur laquelle sont posées des éléctrodes
A multimodal approach to better predict recovery in patients with disorders of consciousness
When a patient is admitted to intensive care due to a disorder of consciousness—such as a coma—establishing their neurological prognosis is a crucial yet challenging task. To reduce the uncertainty that precedes the medical decision, a group of...
05.30.2024 Research, science & health
Population de bactéries commensales (en rouge) dans un intestin grêle de souris. Crédit : University of Chicago
The composition of the gut microbiota could influence decision-making
The way we make decisions in a social context can be explained by psychological, social, and political factors. But what if other forces were at work? Hilke Plassmann and her colleagues from the Paris Brain Institute and the University of Bonn show...
05.16.2024 Research, science & health
See all our news